Christina Ingerslev, Karen Frost & Karen S. Ulrich
We all know that Elvis has left the building
(and Fraiser too). But has Pavlov also left the building?

Recently back home from Clicker Expo in San
Francisco, we continue to puzzle over what seems to be an emerging trend among
some of the presenters at the conference. In traditional learning theory we
view learning from two perspectives; i.e. that of classical conditioning and
operant conditioning. In a number of presentations at the conference we found an
approach to learning that was purely operant, seemingly disregarding the
aspects of learning that are classically conditioned. In fact, one presenter
even argued that we might as well disregard classical conditioning altogether
as all learning, from his point of view, is really operant. This presenter
proceeded to show a video clip of a counter conditioning session (done by
someone else) and argued that the dog’s emotional response was not being
changed but that it was simply learning a new operant response. Another
presenter spoke of establishing the association between a neutral stimulus and
an unconditioned stimulus, in this case done in order to condition a secondary/substitute
reinforcer, as an operant process, that could be viewed “as a behavior that you
can reinforce”. In contrast to this purely operant approach, renowned
presenters like Susan Friedman and Kathy Sdao continue to use the terminology
of both operant and classical conditioning when speaking of the different
aspects of learning and different ways of changing behavior. Hence, the reason
why we feel inclined to ask the question; “Has Pavlov Left the Building?” After
all, a conference on training and learning must be the very place to present
and discuss the various aspects of learning in all its nuances, not a place to
cut corners or trivialize the importance of knowing one’s tools and techniques.

When we train dogs and other animals to
perform a variety of behaviors, our immediate goals are, indisputably, operant.
To put it simply, we wish to teach the animal to perform certain behaviors on
cue. The behaviors are brought under stimulus control so that we can get
reliable performance of the desired behaviors when the learned cues are
presented. However, when we work with animals, it is also important to consider
the emotional state of the learner. Consequently, we cannot disregard the fact
that classical conditioning is continually taking place – while we are busy at
work changing the operant skills of our learner. Also, some aspects of our
training and some of the tools we use are a product of classical conditioning. This goes for our
conditioned marker signals and substitute reinforcers but also associations
between cues and behaviors and between behaviors and reinforcers are
classically conditioned. Cues and behaviors as well as training locations and
training equipment all take on a value for the learner, of either pleasure or
discomfort, depending on the training techniques employed. In our opinion, this
is no small matter and certainly not one to be disregarded when discussing,
planning, or executing training with animals.

We humans like to organize things, e.g.
concepts, into nice and orderly categories. Textbooks on learning distinguish
between the two types of conditioning. However, in real life things are not
quite as neatly organized. Classical and operant conditioning are indeed separate
ways of learning. We know this from studies of how and where these processes
take place in the brain. But in real life learning situations we cannot
separate the two completely or simply choose to disregard the one. Classical
and operant conditioning go hand in hand and in most situations take place at
the same time. What is learned differs, depending on whether you take a
respondent or an operant view of a given situation but they are both part and
parcel of any learning situation in which we look to change the behavior of our
learner. This goes for any training in which the primary objective is to change
the operant behavior of the animal.

When dealing with animals with behavior
issues such as fear and aggression, it is even more important to consider
classical conditioning. Classical conditioning is essential both in order to
understand the conditioned emotional response (CER) that motivates the operant
responses that are considered problematic and in order to know and employ
counter conditioning as a central approach to change the CER and in that way
change the behavioral output that is motivated by the animal’s emotional state.
We do not dispute the fact that operant training can change the emotional state
of an animal through the empowerment gained by acquiring a sense of operant
control in a stressful situation and through the classical conditioning that
takes place when a certain behavior is heavily associated with attractive
reinforcers. But operant conditioning does not do the trick alone, nor does it
suffice to explain all aspects of learning when we teach, train or learn…
We wonder if this tendency to view all
learning from an operant perspective springs from on the one hand a purely Skinnerian
approach to learning and on the other what can be conceived as a limited interest in the motivational
and emotional aspects of behavior.
Denne kommentar er fjernet af forfatteren.
SvarSletkommer artiklen på dansk? Jeg synes, at det er synd at snyde ikke engelsk læsende for disse vise ord. Jeg har mange hundevenner, som ikke læser engelsk fagsprog
SvarSletSynes godt om, Grethe.
SvarSlet